人类学学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 42 ›› Issue (03): 435-444.doi: 10.16359/j.1000-3193/AAS.2023.0037cstr: 32091.14.j.1000-3193/AAS.2023.0037
• 综述 • 上一篇
付卫伟1(), 王晓卫1, 杨晨希2, 王程亮1, 贺树军1, 李保国3()
收稿日期:
2022-08-02
修回日期:
2022-08-02
出版日期:
2023-06-15
发布日期:
2023-06-13
通讯作者:
李保国,教授/博士生导师,主要从事灵长类行为和保护生态学研究。E-mail: 作者简介:
付卫伟,副研究员/博士,主要从事非人灵长类行为生态学研究。E-mail: 基金资助:
FU Weiwei1(), WANG Xiaowei1, YANG Chenxi2, WANG Chengliang1, HE Shujun1, LI Baoguo3()
Received:
2022-08-02
Revised:
2022-08-02
Online:
2023-06-15
Published:
2023-06-13
摘要:
婴幼儿阶段是个体发育的第一个阶段,也是与其照顾者相互作用最为重要的阶段。对个体早期发育阶段行为偏侧的调查,可以帮助我们探索大脑两半球功能偏侧的形成和调控机制,在大脑功能偏侧研究中具有重要意义。本文全面梳理了非人灵长类早期发育阶段所涉及到的3种主要偏侧行为(包括肢体偏好、吸乳偏好和头部转向偏好)以及影响个体偏侧行为的多个潜在影响因子和相关假说,全文以人类相关研究为起点,引出非人灵长类研究现状,以深入了解两者在行为偏侧研究中的异同,为非人灵长类早期发育阶段行为偏侧的研究提供借鉴和参考。
中图分类号:
付卫伟, 王晓卫, 杨晨希, 王程亮, 贺树军, 李保国. 非人灵长类早期发育阶段行为偏侧的研究进展[J]. 人类学学报, 2023, 42(03): 435-444.
FU Weiwei, WANG Xiaowei, YANG Chenxi, WANG Chengliang, HE Shujun, LI Baoguo. Behavioral laterality during the early developmental phase in non-human primates[J]. Acta Anthropologica Sinica, 2023, 42(03): 435-444.
[1] |
MacNeilage PF, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Origins of the left & right brain[J]. Scientific American, 2009, 301(1): 60-67
doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0709-60 pmid: 19555025 |
[2] |
Corballis MC. The evolution and genetics of cerebral asymmetry[J]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2009, 364(1519): 867-879
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0232 URL |
[3] |
Malatesta G, Marzoli D, Prete G, et al. Human lateralization, maternal effects and neurodevelopmental disorders[J]. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 2021, 15: 668520
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.668520 URL |
[4] |
Güntürkün O, Ocklenburg S. Ontogenesis of lateralization[J]. Neuron, 2017, 94(2): 249-263
doi: S0896-6273(17)30153-8 pmid: 28426959 |
[5] |
Regaiolli B, Spiezio C, Hopkins WD. Three actions, two groups: Looking for the origin of primate manual lateralization[J]. Journal of comparative psychology, 2016, 130(3): 259-268
doi: 10.1037/com0000031 pmid: 27124402 |
[6] |
Ghirlanda S, Frasnelle E, Vallortigara G. Interspecific competition and coordination in the evolution of lateralization[J]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2009, 364(1519): 861-866
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0227 URL |
[7] | Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G, Andrew RJ. Divided Brains: The Biology and Behavior of Brain Asymmetries[M]. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2013 |
[8] |
Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization[J]. Behavioural Brain Science, 2005, 28: 575-89
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000105 URL |
[9] |
Michel GF, Babik I, Nelson EL, et al. Evolution and development of handedness: An evo-devo approach[J]. Progress in Brain Research, 2018, 238: 347-374
doi: S0079-6123(18)30052-9 pmid: 30097200 |
[10] | Karenina K, Giljov A, Ingram J, et al. Lateralization of mother-infant interactions in a diverse range of mammal species[J]. Nature Ecology and Evolution, 2017, 0030: 1-5 |
[11] | Annett M. Handedness and brain asymmetry: The right shift theory[M]. Hove: Psychology Press, 2010 |
[12] |
Michel GF, Babik I, Sheu CF, et al. Latent classes in the developmental trajectories of infant handedness[J]. Developmental Psychology, 2014, 50(2): 349-59
doi: 10.1037/a0033312 pmid: 23772820 |
[13] |
Butterworth G, Hopkins B. Origins of handedness in human infants[J]. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1993, 35(2): 177-184
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1993.tb11620.x pmid: 8444331 |
[14] |
Campbell JM, Marcinowski EC, Babik I, et al. The influence of a hand preference for acquiring objects on the development of a hand preference for unimanual manipulation from 6 to 14 months[J]. Infant Behavior and Development, 2015, 39: 107-117
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.013 pmid: 25827261 |
[15] |
Ferre CL, Babik I, Michel GF. Development of infant prehension handedness: A longitudinal analysis during the 6- to 14- month age period[J]. Infant Behavior and Development, 2010, 33(4): 492-502
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.06.002 URL |
[16] |
Ramsay DS. Onset of unimanual handedness in infants[J]. Infant Behavior and Development, 1980, 3: 377-385
doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80045-2 URL |
[17] |
Babik I, Michel GF. Development of role-differentiated bimanual manipulation in infancy: Part 1. The emergence of the skill[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 2016, 58(2): 243-256
doi: 10.1002/dev.21382 pmid: 26644301 |
[18] |
Nelson EL, Campbell JM, Michel GF. Unimanual to bimanual: Tracking the development of handedness from 6 to 24 months[J]. Infant Behavior and Development, 2013, 36(2): 181-188
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.01.009 pmid: 23454419 |
[19] |
Vauclair J. Phylogenetic approach to object manipulation in human and ape infants[J]. Human Development, 2009, 27(5-6): 321-328
doi: 10.1159/000272925 URL |
[20] | Boulinguez-Ambroise G, Pouydebat E, Disarbois E, et al. Maternal cradling bias drives early handedness in infant monkeys: A longitudinal study of grasping lateralization in baboons[J]. bioRxiv, 2021: 434410 |
[21] |
Chorazina H. Shifts in laterality in a baby chimpanzee[J]. Neuropsychologia, 1976, 14(3): 381-384
pmid: 958608 |
[22] |
Cunningham D, Forsythe C, Ward JP. A report of behavioral lateralization in an infant orang-utan (Pongo pygmaeus)[J]. Primates, 1989, 30(2): 249-253
doi: 10.1007/BF02381310 URL |
[23] |
Bard KA, Hopkins WD, Fort CL. Lateral bias in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)[J]. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1990, 104(4): 309-321
pmid: 2282782 |
[24] |
Hopkins WD, Bard KA. Asymmetries in spontaneous head orientation in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)[J]. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1995, 109(4): 808-812
pmid: 7576226 |
[25] |
Hopkins WD, Bard KA. A longitudinal study of hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 2000, 36(4): 292-300
pmid: 10797250 |
[26] |
Hook MA, Rogers LJ. Development of hand preferences in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and effects of aging[J]. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2000, 114(3): 263-271
doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.3.263 pmid: 10994842 |
[27] |
Nelson EL, Emery MS, Babcock SM, et al. Head orientation and handedness trajectory in rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulatta)[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 2011, 53(3): 246-255
doi: 10.1002/dev.20517 pmid: 21400487 |
[28] | Fu WW, Wang XW, Wang CL, et al. Effects of age, sex and manual task on hand preference in wild Rhinopithecus roxellana[J]. Zoological Research, 2019, 40(2): 129-138 |
[29] |
Meguerditchian A, Gardner MJ, Schapiro SJ, et al. The sound of one-hand clapping: Handedness and perisylvian neural correlates of a communicative gesture in chimpanzees[J]. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2012, 279(1735): 1959-1966
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2485 URL |
[30] |
Ward JP, Milliken GW, Dodson DL, et al. Handedness as a function of sex and age in a large population of Lemur[J]. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1990, 104(2): 167-173
pmid: 2364661 |
[31] |
Jaffe BD, Evans TA, Howell S, et al. Left versus right nipple preference in free-ranging infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 2006, 48(3): 266-272
pmid: 16568413 |
[32] |
Packheiser J, Schmitz J, Berretz G, et al. Handedness and sex effects on lateral biases in human cradling: three meta-analyses[J]. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2019, 104: 30-42
doi: S0149-7634(19)30169-1 pmid: 31254542 |
[33] | Salk L. The effects of normal heartbeat sound on the behavior of the newborn infant: Implications for mental health[J]. World Mental Health, 1968, 12: 168-175 |
[34] |
Boulinguez-Ambroise G, Pouydebat E, Disarbois L, et al. Human-like maternal left-cradling bias in monkeys is altered by social pressure[J]. Scientific Reports, 2020, 10: 11036
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68020-3 pmid: 32620801 |
[35] | 高翔, 赵大鹏, 李保国. 非人灵长类动物雌性携抱婴儿与婴儿吸乳偏好的研究进展[J]. 动物学杂志, 2008, 43(6):158-164 |
[36] |
Dienske H, Hopkins B, Reid AK. Lateralization of infant holding in chimpanzees: New data do not confirm previous findings[J]. Behaviour, 1995, 132: 801-809
doi: 10.1163/156853995X00153 URL |
[37] |
Hopkins WD, De Lathouwers M. Left nipple preferences in infant Pan paniscus and P. troglodytes[J]. International Journal of Primatology, 2006, 27(6): 1653-1661
doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9086-4 URL |
[38] |
Nishida T. Left nipple suckling preference in wild chimpanzees[J]. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1993, 14(1): 45-51
doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(93)90017-C URL |
[39] |
Deets AC, Harlow HF. Nipple preferences in nursing singleton- and twin-reared rhesus monkey infants[J]. Developmental Psychology, 1970, 2(2): 159-162
doi: 10.1037/h0028717 URL |
[40] |
Tomaszycki M, Cline C, Griffin B, et al. Maternal cradling and infant nipple preferences in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 1998, 32(4): 305-312
pmid: 9589219 |
[41] |
Tanaka I. Change of nipple preference between successive offspring in Japanese macaques[J]. American Journal of Primatology, 1989, 18(4): 321-325
doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350180406 pmid: 31964028 |
[42] |
Zhao DP, Gao X, Li BG. First wild evidence of neonate nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality in Old World monkeys: A preliminary study from Rhinopithecus roxellana[J]. Behavioural Processes, 2008, 77(3): 364-368
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2007.10.004 URL |
[43] |
Fagot J, Bard KA. Asymmetric grasping response in neonate chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)[J]. Infant Behavior and Development, 1995, 18(2): 253-255
doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(95)90054-3 URL |
[44] |
Damerose E, Hopkins WD. Scan and focal sampling: Reliability in the maternal cradling and infant nipple preferences of olive baboons (Paio anubis)[J]. Animal Behaviour, 2002, 63(3): 511-518
doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1931 URL |
[45] |
Rogers LJ, Kaplan G. Teat preference for suckling in common marmosets: Relationship to side of being carried and hand preference[J]. Laterality, 1998, 3(3): 269-281
pmid: 15513089 |
[46] | Yuma K. Development Of Nipple Suckling Preference in Infant Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata)[C]. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press, 2016 |
[47] | 张鹏, 杨北成, 伍乘风. 海南猕猴行为偏好性研究[J]. 河南师范大学学报:自然科学版, 2017, 45(4): 29-36 |
[48] | Guo YM, Garber PA, Tian JD, et al. Neonatal nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality in wild Taihangshan macaques (Macaca mulatta tcheliensis)[J]. American Journal of Primatology, 2020, 2020: e23197 |
[49] | Michel GF, Harkins DA. Postural and lateral asymmetries in the ontogeny of handedness during infancy[J]. Developmental Psychology, 1986, 19(3): 247-58 |
[50] |
Michel GF. Right-handedness: A consequence of infant supine head-orientation preference[J]. Science, 1981, 212(4495): 685-687
pmid: 7221558 |
[51] |
Provin KA. Early infant motor asymmetries and handedness: A critical evaluation of the evidence[J]. Developmental Neuropsychology, 1992, 8(4): 325-365
doi: 10.1080/87565649209540531 URL |
[52] | Young G, Segalowitz SJ, Corter C.M, et al. Manual Specialization and The Developing Brain: An Review[M]. New York: Academic Press, 1983 |
[53] |
Westergaard GC, Byrne G, Suomi SJ. Early lateral bias in tufted capuchins (Cebus apella)[J]. Developmental Psychobiology, 1998, 32(1): 45-50
pmid: 9452907 |
[54] |
Ocklenburg S, Beste C, Güntürkün O. Handedness: A neurogenetic shift of perspective[J]. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2013, 37(10): 2788-2793
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.014 URL |
[55] |
Carter-Saltzman L. Biological and sociocultural effects on handedness: comparison between biological and adoptive families[J]. Science, 1980, 209(4462): 1263-1265
pmid: 7403887 |
[56] |
Spiegler BJ, Yeni-Komshian GH. Incidence of left-handed writing in a college population with reference to family patterns of hand preference[J]. Neuropsychologia, 1983, 21(6): 651-659
pmid: 6664484 |
[57] |
McKeever WF. A new family handedness sample with findings consistent with X-linked transmission[J]. British Journal of Psychology, 2000, 91(1): 21-39
doi: 10.1348/000712600161655 URL |
[58] |
Jones GV, Martin M. Confirming the X-linked handedness gene as recessive, not additive: Reply to Corballis[J]. Psychological Review, 2001, 108(4): 811-813
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.811 URL |
[59] | Annett M. Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory[M]. London: Psychology Press, 1985 |
[60] |
Matoba M, Masataka N, Tanioka Y. Cross-generational continuity of hand-use preferences in marmosets[J]. Behaviour, 1991, 117(3): 281-286
doi: 10.1163/156853991X00580 URL |
[61] | Westergaard GC, Suomi SJ. Lateral bias in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Concordance between parents and offspring[J]. 2015, 31(2): 143-147 |
[62] |
Westergaard GC, Lussier ID, Higley JD. Familial influences on hand preference: Genotypic variation between closely related primate species[J]. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2001, 20(3): 605-617
pmid: 12002096 |
[63] |
Fu WW, Wang CL, Ren Y, et al. Hand preference in Rhinopithecus roxellana infants: Is it influenced by familial inheritance?[J]. Symmetry, 2020, 12(11): 1905
doi: 10.3390/sym12111905 URL |
[64] | Brooker RJ, Lehman RAW, Heimbuch RC, et al. Hand usage in a colony of bonnett monkeys, Macaca radiata[J]. Behavior Genetica, 1981, 11(1): 49-56 |
[65] |
Byrne RW, Byrne JM. Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. berengei)[J]. Cortex, 1991, 27(4): 521-536
pmid: 1782788 |
[66] | Hopkins WD. Heritability of hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence from a partial interspecies cross-fostering study[J]. Joural of Comparative Psychology, 1999, 113(3): 307-313 |
[67] |
Gesell A, Awes LB. The development of handedness[J]. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1947, 70(2): 155-175
pmid: 20260602 |
[68] | Giesecke M. The genesis of hand preference[J]. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1936, 1(5): 102 |
[69] | Hopkins WD, Bard KA. The ontogeny of lateralized behavior in nonhuman primates with special reference to chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)[A]. In: Ward JP, Hopkins WD (Eds.). Primate Laterality: Current Behavioral Evidence of Primate Asymmetries[A]. New York: Springer, 1993, 251-265 |
[70] |
Hopkins WD. Hand preferences for simple reaching in juvenile chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Continuity in development[J]. Developmental Psychology, 1995, 31(4): 619-625
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.619 URL |
[71] |
Nathan M, Anat BS, Rachel G. Transient motor asymmetry among infants with congenital torticollisd description, characterization, and results of follow-up[J]. Pediatric Neurology, 2016, 59: 36-40
doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.02.005 URL |
[72] |
Ocklenburg S, Bürger C, Westermann C, et al. Visual experience affects handedness[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 2010, 207(2): 447-451
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.10.036 pmid: 19895851 |
[73] |
Adolphs R, Jansari A, Tranel D. Hemispheric perception of emotional valence from facial expressions[J]. Neuropsychology, 2001, 15(4): 516-524
pmid: 11761041 |
[74] |
Ahern GL, Schwartz GE. Differential lateralization for positive vs negative emotion[J]. Neuropsychologia, 1979, 17(6): 693-698
pmid: 522984 |
[75] |
Gainotti G. Emotional behavior and hemispheric side of the lesion[J]. Cortex, 1972, 8(1): 41-55
pmid: 5031258 |
[76] |
Gainotti G. Unconscious processing of emotions and the right hemisphere[J]. Neuropsychologia, 2012, 50(2): 205-218
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.005 pmid: 22197572 |
[77] |
Bourne V, Todd BK. When left means right: An explanation of the left cradling bias in terms of right hemisphere specializations[J]. Developmental Science, 2004, 7(1): 19-24
pmid: 15323114 |
[78] |
Hendriks AW, van Rijswijk M, Omtzigt D. Holding-side influences on infant’s view of mother’s face[J]. Laterality, 2011, 16(6): 641-655
doi: 10.1080/13576500903468904 URL |
[79] |
Huheey JE. Concerning the origin of handedness in humans[J]. Behavior Genetics, 1977, 7(1): 29-32
pmid: 843315 |
[80] | de Chateau P. Clinical application of asymmetrical behavioral communication in parent-infant interaction[A]. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Yogman MW (Eds). Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics[M]. New York: Plenum Press, 1991, 185-208 |
[81] |
Hopkins WD. Laterality in maternal cradling and infant positional biases: implications for the development and evolution of hand preferences in nonhuman primates[J]. International Journal of Primatology, 2004, 25(6): 1243-1265
doi: 10.1023/B:IJOP.0000043961.89133.3d pmid: 18049716 |
[1] | 曾浩然, 刘康康, 罗亚平. 指纹皱纹研究的现状及展望[J]. 人类学学报, 2024, 43(03): 518-528. |
[2] | 何嘉宁, 冉智宇. 中国史前人类的头骨变形[J]. 人类学学报, 2023, 42(05): 575-589. |
[3] | 孙蕾, 李彦桢, 武志江. 河南郑州站马屯遗址仰韶晚期人骨的颅面形态[J]. 人类学学报, 2023, 42(03): 331-341. |
[4] | 王邦彦, 王久存, 文少卿. 古代强直性脊柱炎的诊断标准及国内研究回顾[J]. 人类学学报, 2023, 42(03): 422-434. |
[5] | 刘鑫, 张兴华, 宇克莉, 刘艳霞, 包金萍, 郑连斌. 生物电阻抗法测定广西京族的体成分[J]. 人类学学报, 2022, 41(06): 1028-1036. |
[6] | 沙仁高娃, 程慧珍, 韦兰海. 达斡尔语分支早期在蒙古语族中的地位[J]. 人类学学报, 2022, 41(06): 1037-1046. |
[7] | 宋焕庭, 唐玮, 张丽梅, 张忠良, 张嘉宇, 陈世韬. 指纹与年龄相关性的量化分析[J]. 人类学学报, 2022, 41(06): 1047-1057. |
[8] | 邢松. 现代人出现和演化的化石证据[J]. 人类学学报, 2022, 41(06): 1069-1082. |
[9] | 饶慧芸. 古蛋白质分析在东亚古人类演化中的应用前景[J]. 人类学学报, 2022, 41(06): 1083-1096. |
[10] | 李伟东, 阳高金, 吴琛帆, 杨斌, 张鹏. 非人灵长类声音通讯的研究进展[J]. 人类学学报, 2021, 40(04): 721-732. |
[11] | 李珊, 王文佳, 宇克莉, 郑连斌. 湖南、湖北、贵州土家族成人的体成分比较[J]. 人类学学报, 2021, 40(02): 272-280. |
[12] | 向小雪, 杜慧敏, 宇克莉, 郑连斌. 门巴族、珞巴族与夏尔巴人身体成分特点及比较[J]. 人类学学报, 2021, 40(01): 109-117. |
[13] | Vijaya Lakshmi Pavani MOLLI, Anubhav JAIN, 傅江南, 吴英杰, Jian Q.FENG, 王谦. 树鼩的骨元和骨细胞穴微观结构超微影像的定性比较[J]. 人类学学报, 2020, 39(04): 564-575. |
[14] | 丁振芳, 郝静, 徐华林, 张鹏. 猕猴觅食策略的地域性差异[J]. 人类学学报, 2020, 39(03): 495-506. |
[15] | 吴清禄;伍乘风;张鹏. 非人灵长类的自我觉察能力综述[J]. 人类学学报, 2015, 34(02): 267-280. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||